The artification hypothesis and its relevance to cognitive science

Aus de_evolutionary_art_org
Version vom 11. November 2014, 10:57 Uhr von Gbachelier (Diskussion | Beiträge) (Die Seite wurde neu angelegt: „ == Reference == Ellen Dissanayake: The artification hypothesis and its relevance to cognitive science, evolutionary aesthetics, and neuroaesthetics. Specia…“)

(Unterschied) ← Nächstältere Version | Aktuelle Version (Unterschied) | Nächstjüngere Version → (Unterschied)
Wechseln zu: Navigation, Suche


Reference

Ellen Dissanayake: The artification hypothesis and its relevance to cognitive science, evolutionary aesthetics, and neuroaesthetics. Special Issue on Aesthetic Cognition. Cognitive Semiotics 5, 148-173.

DOI

Abstract

An ethological description of mother-infant interaction suggests that it was adaptive during human evolution. From their early weeks, infants universally respond to certain affect-laden elements of maternal communication that can be called “proto-aesthetic.” I hypothesize that these capacities and receptivities, which evolved from about 1.7 million years ago to enable mother-infant bonding, became a reservoir of affective mechanisms that could be used subsequently by ancestral humans when they first began to “artify”—that is, when they invented the “arts” as vehicles of ceremonial religious experience. The ethological, evolutionary, and cross-cultural foundations of the Artification Hypothesis challenge current influential ideas in cognitive science, evolutionary aesthetics, and neuroaesthetics about human aesthetic capacities and behavior. The hypothesis offers a broader and deeper view to these fields by emphasizing preverbal, presymbolic, pancultural, cross-modal, supra-modal, participative, temporally organized, affective, and affinitive aspects of aesthetic cognition and behavior. It additionally proposes that artifying is an unrecognized but vital evolved component of human nature.

Extended Abstract

Bibtex

Used References

Alcorta, C. S. & Sosis, R. (2005). Ritual, Emotion and Sacred Symbols: The Evolution of Religion as an Adaptive Complex. Human Nature 16, 323-359.

Atran, S. (2002). In Gods We Trust. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Balter, M. (2009). On the Origin of Art and Symbolism. Science 323 (Feb. 6), 709-711.

Barash, D. P. & Barash, N. R. (2005). Madame Bovary’s Ovaries: A Darwinian Look at Literature. New York: Delacorte Press.

Barsalou, L. (1999a). Perceptual Symbol Systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 577- 609.

Barsalou, L. (1999b). Perceptions of Perceptual Symbols (author’s response to commentary). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, 633-660.

Bednarik, R. G. (2003). The Earliest Evidence of Palaeoart. Rock Art Research 20 (2), 89- 135.

Beebe, B. (1986). Mother-Infant Mutual Influence and Precursors of Self- and Object Representations. In J. Masling (Ed.), Empirical Studies of Psychoanalytic Theories, Vol, II (pp. 27-48). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Beebe, B. & Gerstman, L. (1984). A Method of Defining “Packages” of Maternal Stimulation and Their Functional Significance for the Infant with Mother and Stranger. International Journal of Behavioral Development 7, 423-440.

Beebe, B. & Lachmann, F. (1988). The Contribution of Mother-Infant Mutual Influence to the Origins of Self- and Object Representations. Psychoanalytic Psychology 5(4), 305-337.

Beebe, B, Stern, D. & Jaffe, J. (1979). The Kinesic Rhythm of Mother-Infant Interactions. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Of Speech and Time: Temporal Patterns in Interpersonal Contexts (pp. 23-34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Beebe, B., Alson, D., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S. & Crown, C. (1988). Vocal Congruence in Mother-Infant Play. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 17(3), 245-259.

Boyer, P. (2001). Religion Explained. New York: Basic Books.

Brandt, P. A. (2006). Form and Meaning in Art. In M. Turner (Ed.), The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity (pp. 171-188). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Brody, J. J. (1977). Mimbres Painted Pottery. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.

Brown, S. & Dissanayake E. (2009).The Arts are More than Aesthetics: Neuroaesthetics as Narrow Aesthetics. In M. Skov & O. Varshanian (Eds.), Neuroaesthetics (pp.43-57). Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Burghardt, G. M. (2008). Evolution and Paradigms in the Study of Literature. Style 42(3,4), 144-149.

Cacioppo, T. & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection.

Campbell, S. F. (2002). The Art of Kula. Oxford and New York: Berg.

Caporael, L. R. (1997). The Evolution of Truly Social Cognition: The Core Configuration Model. Personality and Social Psychology Review 1, 276-298.

Corballis, M. C. (2002). From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Crawford, C. (1998). The Theory of Evolution in the Study of Human Behavior: An Introduction and Overview. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications (pp. 3-41). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.169

Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Human Brain. London: Allen Lane.

Deacon, T. (2006). The Aesthetic Faculty. In M. Turner (Ed.), The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity (pp. 21-53). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dissanayake, E. (1974). An Hypothesis of the Evolution of Art from Play. Leonardo 7(3), 27-31.

Dissanayake, E. (1979). An Ethological View of Ritual and Art in Human Evolutionary History. Leonardo 12(1), 27-31.

Dissanayake, E. (1984). Does Art Have Selective Value? Empirical Studies of the Arts 2(1), 35-49.

Dissanayake, E. (1990). Of Transcribing and Superliteracy. The World and I, October, 575-587.

Dissanayake, E. (2000). Art and Intimacy: How the Arts Began. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

Dissanayake, E. (2008). If Music is the Food of Love, What About Survival and Reproductive Success? Musicæ Scientiæ Special Issue, 169-195.

Dissanayake, E. (2009). Root, Leaf, Blossom, or Bole: In S.Malloch & C. Trevarthen, (Eds.), Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship (pp. 1730). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

Donald, M. (2006). Art and Cognitive Evolution. In M. Turner (Ed.), The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity (pp. 3-20). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dutton, D. (2009). The Art Instinct: Beauty, Pleasure, and Human Evolution. New York: Bloomsbury.

Eimas, P. D. (1984). Infant Competence and the Acquisition of Language. In D. Caplan, A. Roch Lecours & A. Smith (Eds.), Biological Perspectives on Language (pp. 109-129). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Falk, D. (2009). Finding Our Tongues: Mothers, Infants & The Origins of Language. New York: Basic Books.

Fernald, A. (1992). Maternal Vocalizations to Infants as Biologically Relevant Signals: An Evolutionary Perspective. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 391-428). New York: Oxford University Press.

Field, T. M., Sostek, A. M., Vietze, P., & Leiderman, P. H., eds. (1981) Culture and Early Interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R. & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and Imitation of Facial Expressions by Neonates. Science 218, 179-181.

Gardner, H. (1983) Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (1999) Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century. New York: Basic Books.170

Garfinkel, A.P., Marcom, G. & Schiffman, R. A. (2003). Culture Crisis and Rock Art Intensification: Numic Ghost Dance Paintings and Coso Representational Petroglyphs. In D. Christensen & P. Whitehead (Eds.), American Indian Rock Art 33, 83- 103. Tucson, AZ: American Rock Art Research Association.

Gintis, H. (2000). Strong Reciprocity and Human Sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 206, 169-179.

Gratier, M. & Apter-Danon, G. (2009). The Improvised Musicality of Belonging: Repetition and Variation in Mother-Infant Vocal Interaction. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship (301-327). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Greenspan. S. (1997). The Growth of the Mind and the Endangered Origins of Intelligence. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Gould, S. J. (1977) Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hallpike, C. R. (1979). The Foundations of Primitive Thought. Oxford: Clarendon.

Hamilton, W. (1964). The Genetical Evolution of Social Behaviour. Parts I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7, 1-52.

Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hinde, R. A. (1982). Ethology: Its Nature and Relations With Other Sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jakobson, R. 1971. The Dominant. In L. Matajka & K. Pomorska (Eds. & Trans.), Readings in Russian Poetics: Formalist and Structuralist Views (pp. 82-93). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of Dialogue in Infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 265, 66(2), 1-151.

King, B. J. (2004). The Dynamic Dance: Nonvocal Communication in African Great Apes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.

Lakoff, G. & Turner, M. (1989). More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Leakey, R. (1994). The Origin of Humankind. New York: Basic Books.

Leiderman, P. H., Tulkin, S. R. & Rosenfeld, A., eds. (1977). Culture and Infancy. New York: Academic Press.

Lewkowicz, D. J. & Turkewitz, G. (1980). Cross-Modal Equivalence in Early Infancy: Auditory-Visual Intensity Matching. Developmental Psychology 16(6), 597-607.

Longhi, E. (2003). The Temporal Structure of Mother-Infant Interactions in Musical Contexts. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh.

Lynch, M. P., Kimbrough-Oller, D., Steffens, M. L. & Buder, E. H. (1995). Phrasing in Pre-Linguistic Vocalizations. Developmental Psychobiology 28(1), 3-25.

Malinowski, B. (1925). The Problem of Meaning in Primitive Languages. In C. K. Ogden & I. A. Richards (Eds.), The Meaning of Meaning: A Study of the Influence of Language Upon Thought and of the Science of Symbolism (pp. 451-510). New York: Harcourt Brace.

Malloch, S. & Trevarthen, C., eds. (2009) Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Marks, L. E., Hammeal, R. J. and Bornstein, M. H. (1987). Perceiving Similarity and Comprehending Metaphor. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 52 (1), 1-92.

Martindale, C., Locher, P., & Petrov, V.M. (Eds.). (2006). Evolutionary and Neurocognitive Approaches to Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts. Amityville, NY: Baywood.

Masataka, N. (1996). Perception of Motherese in a Signed Language by 6-Month-Old Deaf Infants. Developmental Psychology 32(5), 874-879.

Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature 201, 1145-1147. Meltzoff A. N. & Moore, M. H. (1977). Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates. Science 219, 1347-1349.

Miall, D. & Dissanayake, E. (2002). The Poetics of Babytalk. Human Nature 14 (4), 337- 364.

Miller, G.F. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Doubleday.

Mukarovský, J. (1964). Standard Language and Poetic Language. In P. L. Garvin (Ed.), A Prague School Reader on Esthetics, Literary Structure, and Style (pp. 17-30). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. (Original work published 1932.)

Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Réserbet-Planty, D. (1999). Expectancies for Social Contingency in 2-Month-Olds. Developmental Science 2, 164-173.

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Papoušek, M. & Papoušek, H. (1981). Musical Elements in the Infant’s Vocalization: Their Significance for Communication, Cognition, and Creativity. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. K. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances In Infancy Research 1 (pp.163-224). Norwood NJ: Ablex.

Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic Epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: Norton.

Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Viking.

Pinker S. & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The Faculty of Language: What’s Special About It? Cognition 95(2), 201.

Portmann, A. (1941). Die Tragzeit der Primaten und die Dauer der Schwangerschaft beim Menschen: Ein Problem der vergleichende Biologie. Revue Suisse de Zoologie 48, 511-518.

Ramachandran, V. S. & Hirstein, W. (1999). The Science of Art. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 15-51.

Richerson, P. & Boyd, R. (1998). The Evolution of Human Ultra-Sociality. In I. Eibl- Eibesfeldt & F. K. Salter (Eds.), Indoctrinability, Ideology and Warfare: Evolutionary Perspectives (pp. 71-95). New York: Berghahn.

Rossano, M. (2007). Supernaturalizing Social Life: Religion and the Evolution of Human Cooperation. Human Nature 18, 272-294.

Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Neuroendocrinology of the Stress Response. In J. R. Becker, S. M. Breedlove & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral Endocrinology (pp. 287-324). Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Scherer, K. R. & Zentner, M. R. (2001). Emotional Effects of Music: Production Rules. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (pp. 361-392). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Shklovsky, V. 1965. Art as Technique. In L. T. Lemon & M. J. Reis (Eds. & Trans.), Russian Formalist Criticism: Four Essays (pp. 3-24). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. (Original work published in 1917).

Skov, M. & Vartanian, O., Eds. (2009). Neuroaesthetics. Amityville NY: Baywood.

Slingerland, E. (2008). What Science Offers the Humanities: Integrating Body and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sober, E. & and Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behaviors. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Sterelny, K. (1996). The Return of the Group. Philosophy of Science 63, 562-584.

Stern, D. (1971). A Microanalysis of Mother-Infant Interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 10, 501-517.

Stern, D. (1985) The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books.

Stern, D. (1999). Vitality Contours: The Temporal Contour of Feelings as a Basic Unit for Construction the Infant’s Social Experience. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life (pp. 67-90). Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum.

Taçon, P. S. C.(1983). Dorset Art in Relation to Prehistoric Culture Stress. Études Inuit/Inuit Studies 7(1), 41-65.

Taçon, P. S. C. & Brockwell, S. (1995). Arnhem Land Prehistory in Landscape, Stone, and Paint. Antiquity 69, 676-695.

Taçon, P. S. C., Wilson, M. & Chippindale, C. (1996). Birth of the Rainbow Serpent i Arnhem Land Rock Art and Oral History. Archaeology Oceania 31, 103-124.

Taylor, S. E. (1992). The Tending Instinct: How Nurturing is Essential for Who We Are and How We Live. New York: Times Books.

Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian Aesthetics Informs Traditional Aesthetics. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary Aesthetics (pp. 9-35). Berlin: Springer.

Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992). The Psychological Foundations of Culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 19-136). Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1994). Infant Semiosis. In W. Noth (Ed.), Origins of Semiosis (pp. 219252). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Trevarthen, C. (1997). Fetal and Neonatal Psychology: Intrinsic Motives and Learning Behavior. In F. Cockburn (Ed.), Advances in Perinatal Medicine (pp. 282-291). New York: Parthenon Publishing Group.

Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-Offspring Conflict. American Zoologist 14, 249-264.

Turner, F. (1985). The Neural Lyre: Poetic Meter, the Brain, and Time. In F. Turner (Ed.), Natural Classicism: Essays on Literature and Science (pp. 61-108). Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia.

Turner, M. (2006). The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Turner, V. (1969). The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

van Gennep, A. (1960). The Rites of Passage. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (original publication 1909)

Voland, E. & Grammer, K., Eds. (2003). Evolutionary Aesthetics. Berlin: Springer.

Wade, N. (2006). Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of our Ancestors. New York: Penguin Press.

Wagner, S., Winner, E., Cicchetti, D. & Gardner, H. (1981). ‘Metaphorical’ Mapping in Human Infants, Child Development 52, 728-731.

Waterman, R. (1955). Music in Australian Aboriginal Culture: Some Sociological and Psychological Implications, Journal of Music Therapy 5, 40-49.

Watson, D. & Clark, L. A. (1994). The Vicissitudes of Mood: A Schematic Model. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (pp. 400-405). New York: Oxford University Press.

Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s Cathedral. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Zeki, S. (1999). Inner Vision. An Exploration of Art and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.


Links

Full Text

http://ellendissanayake.com/publications/pdf/EllenDissanayake_CognitiveSemiotics5.pdf

intern file

Sonstige Links