The arts are more than aesthetics: Neuroaesthetics as narrow aesthetics
Inhaltsverzeichnis
Reference
Ellen Dissanayake, Steven Brown: The arts are more than aesthetics: Neuroaesthetics as narrow aesthetics. In Martin Skov & Oshin Vartanian (eds.), Neuroaesthetics. Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 43-57.
DOI
Abstract
Neuroaesthetics is a young enough field that there seems to be no established view of its proper subject matter. Morphologically, the term implies the scientific study of neural aspects of the perception of artworks such as paintings, or elements of artworks such as musical intervals. We are concerned, however, that practitioners of this new field may not be aware of the tremendous ambiguities inherent in the terms “aesthetics” and “art,” ones that limit a proper understanding of human art behavior. Connotations of these terms are particularly inappropriate and mis- leading when considering the experiences, practices, and functions of the arts in preindustrial, folk, aboriginal, or Pleistocene societies, and even in contemporary popular culture. It is only during the last two centuries that the terms “Art” (with an implied capital A, connoting an independent realm of prestigious and revelatory works) and “aesthetics” (as a unique, and even reverential, mode of attention toward such works) have taken on their present elitist meanings and become unavoidably inter- twined (Davies, 2006; Shiner, 2001). The word “aesthetic” (from the Greek aiesthesis, having to do with the senses) was first used in 1735 by a German philosopher in a book on poetry (Baumgarten, 1735/1954), and since that time has been employed in two different, but not always distinct, ways. Enlightenment philosophers and their followers gradually developed the now elitist notion of “the aesthetic”—a special form of disinterested knowledge and appreciation—to describe the emotional response elicited by the perception of great works of art (Shiner, 2001). While this meaning of aesthetic has strong historical connections with the arts and with artworks, a second usage has come to refer to any value system having to do with the appreciation of beauty, such as the beauty of nature. In recent decades, for example, some ethologists and evolutionary psychologists have adopted this second, broader notion of aesthetics in a new field, originally called “landscape aesthetics” (Appleton, 1990; Orians, 2001; Ruso, Renninger, & Atzwanger, 2003) or “Darwinian aesthetics” (Thornhill, 1998), but generally called “evolutionary aesthetics” (Voland & Grammer, 2003) today. Evolutionary aesthetics investigates sensory preferences in animals and humans that promote selective attention and positive emotional responses toward objects in the environment that lead to adaptive decision making and problem solving (Orians, 2001). Objects perceived in this manner are considered to be beautiful (Thornhill, 1998). Following Darwin (1871), who noted that animals (especially birds) seemed to appreciate beauty and who attributed the spectacular colors and patterns of male birds to female choice, some workers in evolutionary aesthetics have proposed that human art arose by sexual selection in a similar manner to the courtship displays performed by male birds to attract females for mating (e.g., Miller, 2000, 2001).
Extended Abstract
Bibtex
Used References
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E., & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537-551.
Alcorta, C. S., & Sosis, R. (2005). Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: The evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Human Nature, 16, 323-359.
Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Stappen, I., Panitz, D., Ghahremani, D. G., Glover, G., et al. (2003). Dissociated neural representation of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 196-202.
Appleton, J. (1990). The symbolism of habitat. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage, 21, 1155-1166.
Baumgarten, A. (1735/1954). Reflections on poetry. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Beebe, B., Lachmann, F. L., & Jaffe, J. (1997). Mother-infant interaction structures and presymbolic self and object representations. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 7, 133-182.
Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818-11823.
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382-387.
Brown, S. (2000). Evolutionary models of music: From sexual selection to group selection. In F. Tonneau & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. 13: Behavior, evolution and culture (pp. 231-281). New York: Plenum.
Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., & Parsons, L. M. (2004). Passive music listening spontaneously engages limbic and paralimbic systems. NeuroReport, 15, 2033-2037.
Brown, S., & Volgsten, U. (2006). Is Mozart’s music good? In S. Brown & U. Volgsten (Eds.), Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music (pp. 365-369). New York: Berghahn.
Burgdorf, J., & Panksepp, J. (2006). The neurobiology of positive emotions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 173-187.
Carter, C. S., Lederhandler, I. I., & Kirkpatrick, B. (Eds.). (1999). The integrative neuro- biology of affiliation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chernoff, J. M. (1979). African rhythm and African sensibility. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions require cognition. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 181-191). New York: Oxford University Press.
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never? In L. Nadel, R. Lane, & G. L. Ahern (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24-61). New York: Oxford University Press.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: J. Murray.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotion in man and animals. London: J. Murray.
Davies, S. (2006). The philosophy of art. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Dissanayake, E. (1988). What is art for? Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Dissanayake, E. (2000). Art and intimacy: How the arts began. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Dissanayake, E. (2006). Ritual and ritualization: Musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals. In S. Brown & U. Volgsten (Eds.), Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music (pp. 31-57). Oxford: Berghahn.
Dissanayake, E. (2007). What art is and what art does: An overview of contemporary evolutionary hypotheses. In C. Martindale, P. Locher, & V. Petrov (Eds.), Evolutionary and neurocognitive approaches to aesthetics, creativity, and the arts (pp. 1-14). Amityville, NY: Baywood.
Dissanayake, E. (in press). If music is the food of love, what about survival and repro- ductive success? Musicae Scientiae.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550-553.
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cog- nition and emotion (pp. 45-60). Sussex, UK: John Wiley and Sons.
Falk, D. (1998). Hominin brain evolution: Looks can be deceiving. Science, 280, 1714.
Francis, S., Rolls, E. T., Bowtell, R., McGlone, F., O’Doherty, J., Browning, A., et al. (1999). The representation of pleasant touch in the brain and its relationship with taste and olfactory areas. NeuroReport, 10, 453-459.
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hennelotter, A., & Schroeder, U. (2006). Partially dissociable neural substrates for recog- nizing basic emotions: A critical review. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 443-456.
Kawabata, H., & Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 1699-1705.
Miall, D. S., & Dissanayake, E. (2003). The poetics of babytalk. Human Nature, 14, 337-364.
Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.
Miller, G. (2001). Aesthetic fitness: How sexual selection shaped artistic virtuosity as a fitness indicator and aesthetic preferences as mate choice criteria. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 2, 20-25.
Miller, W. B., & Rodgers, J. L. (2001). The ontogeny of human bonding systems: Evolutionary origins, neural bases, and psychological mechanisms. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interaction between two month-olds and their mothers. In T. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177-197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Réserbet-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164-173.
Nakamura, K., Nagumo, S., Ito, K., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., Nakamura, A., et al. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of the assessment of facial attractiveness. NeuroReport, 9, 753-757.
Nelson, E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: Contri- butions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinepherine, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 437-452.
Nitschke, J. B., Nelson, E. E., Rusch, B. D., Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R. & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infant. NeuroImage, 21, 583-592.
O’Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M. & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41, 147-155.
Orians, G. H. (2001). An evolutionary perspective on aesthetics. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 2(1), 25-29.
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 315-331.
Pedersen, C. A., Caldwell, J. D., Jirikowski, G. F., & Insel, T. R. (Eds.). (1992). Oxytocin in maternal, sexual and social behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 652.
Portmann, A. (1941). Die Tragzeit der Primaten und die Dauer der Schwangerschaft beim Menschen: Ein Problem der vergleichende Biologie. Revue Suisse de Zoologie, 48, 511-518.
Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. London: Cambridge University Press.
Reisenzein, R. (1994). Pleasure-arousal theory and the intensity of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 525-539.
Rolls, E. T. (2004). Convergence of sensory systems in the orbitofrontal cortex in primates and brain design for emotion. The Anatomical Record, 281A, 1212-1225.
Rolls, E. T. (2005). Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the control of food intake. Physiology and Behavior, 85, 45-56.
Ruso, B., Renninger, L., & Atzwanger, K. (2003). Human habitat preferences: A generative territory for evolutionary aesthetics research. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 279-294). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shiner, L. (2001). The invention of art: A cultural history. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Small, D. M., Gregory, M. D., Mak, E. Y., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M. M., & Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron, 39, 701-711.
Solso, R. L. (1994). Cognition and the visual arts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Stockfelt, O. (2006). Music and reuse: Theoretical and historical considerations. In S. Brown & U. Volgsten (Eds.), Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music (pp. 315-335). New York: Berghahn.
Thornhill, R. (1998). Darwinian aesthetics. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, applications (pp. 543-572). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived activities: Their causation, biological significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27, 1-32.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321-347). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Tucker, D. M. (1992). Developing emotions and cortical networks. In M. R. Gunnar & C. A. Nelson (Eds.), Development, behavior, neuroscience (pp. 75-128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Turner, V. (1969). The ritual process: Structure and anti-structure. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Üvnas-Moberg, K. (1999). Physiological and endocrine effects of social contact. In C. S. Carter, I. I. Lederhendler, & B. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), The integrative neurobiology of affiliation (pp. 245-261). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
van Damme, W. (1996). Beauty in context: Towards an anthropological approach to aesthetics. Leiden: E. J. Brill.
Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2004). Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. NeuroReport, 15, 893-897.
Voland, E., & Grammer, K. (Eds.). (2003). Evolutionary aesthetics. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1048-1054.
Zeki, S. (1999). Inner vision: An exploration of art and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Links
Full Text
http://ellendissanayake.com/publications/pdf/Brown_Dissanayake.pdf