The science of art: a neurological theory of aesthetic experience

Aus de_evolutionary_art_org
Wechseln zu: Navigation, Suche

Reference

Ramachandran, V., Hirstein, W.: The science of art: a neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(1–2), 15–52 (1999).

DOI

Abstract

We present a theory of human artistic experience and the neural mechanisms that mediate it. Any theory of art (or, indeed, any aspect of human nature) has to ideally have three components. (a) The logic of art: whether there are universal rules or principles; (b) The evolutionary rationale: why did these rules evolve and why do they have the form that they do; (c) What is the brain circuitry involved? Our paper begins with a quest for artistic universals and proposes a list of ‘Eight laws of artistic experience ’ — a set of heuristics that artists either consciously or unconsciously deploy to optimally titillate the visual areas of the brain. One of these principles is a psychological phenomenon called the peak shift effect: If a rat is rewarded for discriminating a rectangle from a square, it will respond even more vigorously to a rectangle that is longer and skinnier that the prototype. We suggest that this principle explains not only caricatures, but many other aspects of art. Example: An evocative sketch of a female nude may be one which selectively accentuates those feminine form-attributes that allow one to discriminate it from a male figure; a Boucher, a Van Gogh, or a Monet may be a caricature in ‘colour space ’ rather than form space. Even abstract art may employ ‘supernormal ’ stimuli to excite form areas in the brain more

Extended Abstract

Bibtex

Used References

Allman, J.M. and Kaas, J.H. (1971), ‘Representation of the visual field in striate and adjoining cortex of the owl monkey’, Brain Research, 35, pp. 89–106.

Arnheim, R. (1956), Art and Visual Perception (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press). Attneave, F. (1954), ‘Some informational aspects of visual perception’, Psychological Review, 61, pp. 183–93.

Barlow, H.B. (1986), ‘Why have multiple cortical areas?’, Vision Research, 26 (1), pp. 81–90.

Bauer, R.M. (1984), ‘Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: a neuropsychological application of the Guilty Knowledge Test’, Neuropsychologia, 22, pp. 457–69.

Churchland, P.S., Ramachandran, V.S. and Sejnowski, T.J. (1994), ‘A critique of pure vision’, in Large-scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain, ed. C. Koch and J.L. Davis (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press).

Crick, F. and Koch, C. (1998), ‘Consciousness and neuroscience’, Cerebral Cortex, 8 (2), pp. 97–107.

di Pellegrino, G, Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L.; Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. (1992), ‘Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study’, Experimental Brain Research, 91 (1), pp. 176–80.

Gombrich, E.H. (1973), ‘Illusion and art’, in Illusion in Nature and Art, ed. R.L. Gregory and E.H. Gombrich (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons).

Hirstein, W.S. and Ramachandran, V.S. (1997), ‘Capgras Syndrome: A novel probe for understanding the neural representation of the identity and familiarity of persons’, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 264, pp. 437–44.

Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. (1979), ‘Brain mechanisms of vision’, Scientific American, 241, pp. 150–62

Johansson, G. (1975), ‘Visual motion perception’, Scientific American, 232, pp. 76–8. Julesz, B. (1971), Foundations of Cyclopean Perception (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).

Livingstone, M.S. and Hubel, D.H. (1987), ‘Psychophysiological evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement and depth’, Journal of Neuroscience, 7, pp. 3416–68. Marr, D. (1981), Vision (San Francisco, CA: Freeman and Sons).

Penrose, Roland. (1973), ‘In praise of illusion’, in Illusion in Nature and Art, ed. R.L. Gregory and E.H. Gombrich (New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons).

Pinker, S. (1998), How the Mind Works (New York: William Morrow).

Ramachandran, V.S. (1990), ‘Visual perception in people and machines’, in AI and the Eye, ed. A. Blake and T. Troscianko (Chichester: Wiley).

Ramachandran, V.S. and Hirstein, W. (1997), ‘Three laws of qualia: Clues from neurology about the biological functions of consciousness and qualia’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4 (5–6), pp. 429–57.

Ramachandran, V.S. and Blakeslee, S. (1998), Phantoms in the Brain (New York: William Morrow and Co).

Ramachandran, V.S., Armell, C., Foster, C. and Stoddard, R. (1998), ‘Object recognition can drive apparent motion perception’, Nature, 395, pp. 852–3.

Shepard, R. (1981), In Perceptual Organization, ed. M. Kubovy and T. Pomerantz (New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum).

Singer, W. and Gray, C.M. (1995), ‘Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis’, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, pp. 555–86.

Snyder, A. and Thomas, M. (1997), ‘Autistic savants give clues to cognition’, Perception 26, pp. 93–6.

Tinbergen, N. (1954), Curious Naturalists (New York: Basic Books).

Tovee, M.J., Rolls, E. and Ramachandran V.S. (1996), ‘Rapid visual learning in neurons in the primate visual cortex’, Neuroreport, 7, pp. 2757–60.

Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (1985), ‘Knowledge without awareness: An autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics’, Science, 228, pp. 1453–4.

Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (1988), ‘Non-conscious face recognition in patients with face agnosia’, Behavioral Brain Research, 30, pp. 235–49.

Van Essen, D.C. and Maunsell, J.H. (1980), ‘Two-dimensional maps of the cerebral cortex’, J. Comp. Neurol., 191, pp. 255–81.

Zeki, S. (1980), ‘The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex’, Nature, 284, pp. 412–18.

Zeki, S. (1998), ‘Art and the brain’, Proceedings of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 127 (2), pp. 71–104. Reprinted in Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6 (6–7), pp. 76–96.


Links

Full Text

http://pages.cs.wisc.edu/~dyer/ah336/papers/ramachandran-science-art.pdf

intern file

Sonstige Links

http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.108.2599