Attributing Creative Agency: Are we doing it right?: Unterschied zwischen den Versionen
(→Bibtex) |
(→Bibtex) |
||
Zeile 52: | Zeile 52: | ||
location = {Park City, Utah, USA}, | location = {Park City, Utah, USA}, | ||
pages = {17-22}, | pages = {17-22}, | ||
− | url = {http://computationalcreativity.net/iccc2015/proceedings/1_3Bown.pdf | + | url = {http://computationalcreativity.net/iccc2015/proceedings/1_3Bown.pdf http://de.evo-art.org/index.php?title=Attributing_Creative_Agency:_Are_we_doing_it_right }, |
− | |||
publisher = {International Association for Computational Creativity}, | publisher = {International Association for Computational Creativity}, | ||
keywords = {computational, creativity}, | keywords = {computational, creativity}, |
Aktuelle Version vom 2. November 2015, 11:26 Uhr
Inhaltsverzeichnis
Reference
Oliver Bown: Attributing Creative Agency: Are we doing it right?. In: Computational Creativity 2015 ICCC 2015, 17-22.
DOI
Abstract
When contemplating the creativity of computational systems, a host of factors have been taken into consideration, many of which people have attempted to measure or otherwise operationalise: novelty, value, P-creativity versus H-creativity, exploration versus transformation, the subjective evaluation and contextualisation of the artefact, and so on. Whilst of equal importance, the systematic and rigorous attribution of creative agency to different actors in the production of a specific output has been given less attention. It is common to make the simplifying assumption that the most direct contributor to an artefact is that artefact’s sole author, but arguably this is never the case: all human creativity occurs in the context of networks of mutual influence, including a cumulative pool of knowledge.
This paper looks at how we might better formalise creative authorship such that for any artefact, a set of agents could be precisely attributed with their relative contributions to the existence of that entity. It asks only what the nature of this formalisation should be, and concludes that a more critical approach is needed to the creative agency of human actors, and thus the expected creative agency of machines.
I draw on two critical notions that can inform a methodology for the ascription of creative origins in computational creativity: becoming, and the agency of networks of interaction.
I look at a example from both historical human creativity and computation creativity, to consider how we can break down creative agency and ascribe it to different sources. Practical implications are dicsussed.
Extended Abstract
Bibtex
@inproceedings{ author = {Bown, Oliver}, title = {Attributing Creative Agency: Are we doing it right?}, booktitle = {Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Computational Creativity}, series = {ICCC2015}, year = {2015}, month = {Jun}, location = {Park City, Utah, USA}, pages = {17-22}, url = {http://computationalcreativity.net/iccc2015/proceedings/1_3Bown.pdf http://de.evo-art.org/index.php?title=Attributing_Creative_Agency:_Are_we_doing_it_right }, publisher = {International Association for Computational Creativity}, keywords = {computational, creativity}, }
Used References
Aunger, R. 2000. Darwinizing Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.
Baars, B. J. 2005. Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in brain research 150:45–53.
Barkow, J. H.; Cosmides, L.; and Tooby, J. 1992. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: OUP.
Boden, M. 1990. The Creative Mind. George Weidenfeld and Nicholson Ltd.
Bown, O.; Eigenfeldt, A.; Martin, A.; Carey, B.; and Pasquier, P. 2013. The musical metacreation weekend: challenges arising from the live presentation of musically metacreative systems. In Proceedings of the musical metacreation workshop, AIIDE conference, Boston.
Bown, O. 2012. Generative and adaptive creativity. In Mc- Cormack, J., and d’Inverno, M., eds., Computers and Creativity. Berlin: Springer. 361–381.
Bown, O. 2014. Empirically grounding the evaluation of creative systems: incorporating interaction design. In Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Computational Creativity.
Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press.
Clark, A. 2003. Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford University Press.
Colton, S.; Pease, A.; and Ritchie, G. 2001. The effect of input knowledge on creativity. Technical Reports of the Navy Center for Applied Research in Artificial Intelligence.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1999. Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J., ed., The Handbook of Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press. 313–335.
Dawkins, R., and Krebs, J. R. 1978. Animal signals: Information or manipulation? In Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N. B., eds., Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Sinauer Associates. 282–309.
Eigenfeldt, A. 2014. Generating structure–towards largescale formal generation. In Tenth Artificial Intelligence and Interactive Digital Entertainment Conference.
Ingold, T. 2007. Materials against materiality. Archaeological dialogues 14(01):1–16.
Latour, B. 1996. On actor-network theory: a few clarifications. Soziale welt 369–381.
Law, J. 1992. Notes on the theory of the actor network: Ordering, strategy and heterogeneity. Published online at www.lancs.ac.uk/fass/sociology/papers/law-noteson- ant.pdf.
Malafouris, L. 2007. At the potter’s wheel: An argument for material agency.
Marriott, M. 1976. Hindu transactions: Diversity without dualism. University of Chicago, Committee on Southern Asian Studies.
McCorduck, P. 1990. AARON’s Code: Meta-Art, Artificial Intelligence, and the Work of Harold Cohen. W. H. Freeman and Co.
McCormack, J.; Bown, O.; Dorin, A.; McCabe, J.; Monro, G.; and Whitelaw, M. 2014. Ten questions concerning generative computer art. Leonardo 47(2):135–141.
McLean, A., and Wiggins, G. A. 2010. Bricolage programming in the creative arts. In 22nd Annual Psychology of Programming Interest Group. Miller, D. 2010. Stuff. Polity.
Minsky, M. 1988. Society of mind. Simon and Schuster.
Nia, H. T.; Jain, A. D.; Liu, Y.; Alam, M.-R.; Barnas, R.; and Makris, N. C. 2015. The evolution of air resonance power efficiency in the violin and its ancestors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences 471(2175).
Nietzsche, F. 1998. Twilight of the Idols. Oxford University Press.
Ritchie, G. 2007. Some empirical criteria for attributing creativity to a computer program. Minds and Machines 17(1):67–99.
Shennan, S. 2002. Genes, Memes and Human History. Thames and Hudson, London.
Simonton, D. K. 2003. Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: the integration of product, person, and process perspectives. Psychological bulletin 129(4):475.
Sperber, D. 2007. Seedless grapes: Nature and culture. In Margolis, E., and Laurence, S., eds., Creations of the Mind: Theories of Artefacts and Their Representation. Oxford University Press. chapter 7.
Wiggins, G. A. 2006. A preliminary framework for description, analysis and comparison of creative systems. Knowledge-Based Systems 19(7):449–458.
Links
Full Text
http://computationalcreativity.net/iccc2015/proceedings/1_3Bown.pdf